For a Few Articles More

The two articles that I have chosen to discuss as examples of well written and not so well written articles are both related to paleopathology. The first, a recent article by Sabrina Agarwal (2012), is a piece that I came across earlier this semester in reading for another course. I think that this article provides an excellent example of a work that gently pushes the boundaries of the field in terms of the inclusion of gender-related theory in paleopathological analyses and the according alteration of methods, but in a way that fits easily within the existing and established “canonical” dialogues. The second, written by Debra Martin et al. (2010), is an article that I have loved to hate since I first encountered in at the beginning of my Master’s coursework. While I knew that something about this piece did not sit right with me, it took reading this article with a deeper focus on the way it was written to figure out exactly what that unsettling aspect was.

Agarwal SC. 2012. The past of sex, gender, and health: bioarchaeology of the aging skeleton. American Anthropologist 114: 322-335.

In this article, Agarwal (2012) examines how bioarchaeologists approach the evaluation of osteoporosis or bone loss in the past in relation to sex and age, and how standard approaches in paleopathology that focus on sex-based differences can obscure other sources of variation. This focus on biological sex as the major determining factor for variation in bone loss is related both to the limitations imposed by the current method of determining sex as the first step in standard bioarchaeological methodologies, as well as to the expectations suggested by a modern biomedical paradigm. Using a bioarchaeological case study of rural and urban samples from medieval Britain, the author explores the fluidity of categories like sex and gender in the past, considering them in the context of a lifecourse approach to aging.

The first aspect of the article that makes it a great example of social scientific writing is its structure. Agarwal (2012) lays out the fundamental conclusions of her study and the basic tenets of her argument in the first sentences of her introduction. The article is clearly organized and manages to preserve a consistent flow despite its comprehensive coverage of contextual information in terms of paleopathological methods and previous studies, theoretical developments within anthropology, and the background, methods, results, and interpretations based on her own case study as well. Agarwal (2012) succinctly outlines the way that bioarchaeologists have approached questions of sex and gender, aging, and bone loss in the past, as well as the points at which these and other factors influencing the maintenance and loss of bone intersect in analyses. She also outlines major methodological trends within paleopathology as well as important theoretical developments within this and other fields that have relevance to the conceptualization of aging, gendered identities, and health. She manages to engage in theoretical dialogues outside of paleopathology and physical anthropology. Bone loss in medieval Britain is presented early on as an ideal example of the theoretical issues the author wishes to explore, providing an immediate and obvious answer to the question of what this may be a case of. The “hourglass” format incorporating various levels of analysis is implemented extremely well in this article, with the discussion of major theoretical developments and paleopathological trends giving way to a discussion of the specifics of the author’s chosen case study, and then once again branching out to a discussion of how the case study is able to engage with theoretical issues and contribute to the conception of a “new paradigm” emphasizing cumulative life experiences in the creation of skeletal bodies (Agarwal 2012, 331).

The approach taken in this article truly is novel in paleopathology, for “although theoretical work has sought to critically address the dualities of biological sex and gender, there have been very few studies that have explicitly explored this theoretical ground in conjunction with the analyses of actual skeletal remains” (Agarwal 2012, 323). The article does an excellent job of highlighting the points of friction within the field, acknowledging the lack of correlation between theoretical developments and practical applications. The tendency to collapse the concepts of biological sex and gender, despite the engagement of bioarchaeologists with the concept of gender as socioculturally constructed, is identified as another point of friction.

In her case study, Agarwal (2012) demonstrates that the skeletal samples from rural medieval Britain did not follow a modern pattern of bone loss, and did not demonstrate significant differences based on biological sex. Based on historical, bioarchaeological, and biomedical evidence, the author considers the potential effects of nutrition, physical activity, and reproductive behavior as influencing factors on bone loss in the rural and urban skeletal samples. Agarwal (2012) mentions the resistance she faced from reviewers and editors in attempting to publish the data from these sites in a way that analyzed both biological sexes together rather than separately. This clearly demonstrates the “canonical” resistance to conceptualizing sex, gender, and the aging process in a different way than has been traditionally applied. This may be one of the factors leading the author to present her own modifications to traditional approaches as merely a “subtle change in analysis” (Agarwal 2012, 331) that pushes the boundaries without upsetting the established canons. In this article, the author does an excellent job of highlighting and verbalizing what to this point have been underlying and mainly unconscious assumptions inherent in bioarchaeological analyses of gender and aging. She calls attention to limitations and questions established methods, but in a way that still manages to remain acceptable (even if only barely, by her own admission) to more traditionally oriented researchers. I find this aspect of the article inspiring, and hope that I can manage to accomplish this type of criticism half so well.

There is one problem identified by the author that she does not manage to address in her own work. While Agarwal (2012) is able to identify the limitations imposed by the primacy of sex determination and the exclusion of individuals for whom biological sex cannot be securely established on bioarchaeological views of gender and consideration of gender differences in the past, her own analysis is also affected by this limitation. It is likely that further investigation is necessary to devise a solution to this complicated issue. It is also possible that the incorporation of this methodological step was a necessary inclusion by the author in order to compromise novelty with a more “canonical” way of doing things in order to engage with established dialogues.

Martin DL, Harrod RP, Fields M. 2010. Beaten down and worked to the bone: bioarchaeological investigations of women and violence in the ancient southwest. Landscapes of Violence 1 (Art 3): 1-19.

This paper by Martin et al. (2010) appears to me to represent an attempt on the part of the authors to accomplish the same goal as the article by Agarwal (2012), namely introducing a theoretical concept that is not commonly incorporated in paleopathological analyses and applying it to an osteological case study to demonstrate its utility in bioarchaeology. However, these authors do not manage to connect the theory and practical application strongly enough to demonstrate convincingly that this theoretical concept can be of direct relevance to paleopathologists or that their bioarchaeological sample provides an ideal case study. Martin et al. (2010) present an interesting discussion of various developments in the study of violence, such as the functional nature of violence, the involvement of women in violent practices as perpetrators rather than victims, and differences between direct and indirect violence, the implementation of which may vary by gender. They do a good job of outlining theoretical concepts relating to how women may have contributed to violent practices in past societies. However, as far as I can tell the case study they have chosen provides no specific evidence to suggest that this practice may actually have occurred at the site they discuss. Therefore, while the article raises some interesting possibilities and engages with dialogues that are occurring in other disciplines, the theoretical and practical sections of the article seem completely disparate with nothing strongly tying them together.

In terms of organization and structure, this article displays a tendency toward “mystery writing.” Martin et al. (2010) thoroughly introduce the theoretical concepts that they will be discussing in the first few introductory and contextual sections. However, there is no clear statement of the aims or accomplishments of the paper within the introduction, or in fact until several pages into the article. This is made more difficult by the fact that the structure of articles in this particular journal does not include an abstract, and so there is no concise statement of the authors’ conclusions until the final sections of the paper. The case study chosen by the authors as an illustration of their theoretical concepts is not even mentioned until the fourth page, and is not tied into the major conclusions until much further on in the paper. The authors do attempt to structure the scale of topics in their paper in an hourglass formation, but the various segments are not tied together well enough to create a truly logical flow.

There are also difficulties with this article in terms of the level of support within various bodies of literature for the concepts the authors attempt to apply to their skeletal case study. For example, Martin et al. (2010) state very clearly that survivable injuries impacting the cranial vault inevitably lead to brain damage. They provide no citation to a biomedical or clinical study that might support this claim, yet they go on to state fairly unequivocally that this brain damage would cause behavioral changes relevant to individuals experiencing further violent conflicts. I think that this represents an overextension of the evidence, as it is presented as an established fact rather than one possibility among many. Additionally, the ethnographic comparative example of the Turkana society used by Martin et al. (2010) as supporting evidence is from an extremely different context than that of the American southwest. The authors themselves state that there are many cultural differences between the two societies, and that while female-instigated violence occurs in the context of polygynous marriages among the Turkana, there is no ethnographic evidence for polygyny in the ancient southwest. Furthermore, the source cited for this ethnographic information is an unpublished conference presentation rather than published reference material; as such this information is inaccessible to other researchers who may wish to confirm the validity of the comparison. The only other source of comparison included in the article is data from non-human primates.

The ideas discussed in the article are very interesting, and it would definitely be of benefit for bioarchaeologists to consider these possibilities in their analyses of past violent behaviors; the authors also tap into some useful theoretical developments in terms of different culturally determined definitions of violence. However, I don’t think that this bioarchaeological sample is the best case study in which to examine or apply these theories. The skeletal and archaeological data do not contain any features that particularly suggest a situation in which women are the perpetrators of violence to be relevant, and the association seems therefore to be mainly conjecture. In my opinion, the major conclusions made by Martin et al. (2010) are not fully supported by reliable evidence. It appears as though the authors may be reaching for a case study to try to fit the theory, even though it doesn’t clearly or strongly demonstrate evidence of the type of circumstances they are implicating. As the authors themselves state in their conclusions, “the development of [this] new hypothesis related to the root causes of violence takes a more nuanced approach as well as, at times, an interpretive leap in teasing apart the potential effects of social factors on violent behaviors” (Martin et al. 2010, 14).

Print Friendly, PDF & Email

5 thoughts on “For a Few Articles More

  1. Hi Laura!
    I too found Agarwal’s (2012) paper to be very inspiring. Her paper does a great job of incorporating and discussing theory in an explicit way. I find it refreshing as osteology papers use theory but it’s nearly always hidden in our narratives. I also found her challenge of our, taken for granted, ideas about gender and biological sex very novel. I haven’t yet read the paper by Martin et al. (2010). It does sound interesting. I wonder if they didn’t choose the south west for convenience as I believe Martin has done some research and the south west is known for some of its more violent periods. Though, most places probably went through periods of violence so this isn’t a great excuse. I was curious, did Martin et al. (2010) think their conclusions supported the theory? i.e. positive that their choice in case study was appropriate? I do sympathize as ethnography and theory can provides some great ideas but they can also be very difficult to support in the past.

    • Hi Annabelle,

      Thank you for your feedback! I think those are the exact things that I really liked about Agarwal’s (2012) paper – I think it is so important to discuss the assumptions and uses of theory that are often taken for granted. There needs to be open discussion of these issues in order to better evaluate whether they are being applied effectively.
      I think that Martin et al. (2010) did choose the southwestern example out of convenience. Dr. Martin has done quite a bit of work at sites in this area. It appeared that they did think the results of their case study supported the theory, although they at no point in the article pinpointed the evidence that led them to this conclusion. This may have been the “interpretive leap” that they themselves referred to; however I don’t think this leap was warranted based on the available evidence, or lack thereof. Most of the cases in which I have seen ethnographic evidence used well have applied ethnographic comparisons with groups from a similar area, or with major similarities in cultural or lifestyle factors that would justify the comparison. I think that ethnography would have been convincing supporting evidence in this case had the authors been able to demonstrate enough potential similarity between the two groups.

  2. Hello Laura,
    I have not read either of your articles, but the way you have described Argawal’s is exceptional. Do you feel that you really enjoyed her article because of her inherent presence in her own work? The way you have described it suggests that her voice was not only present throughout her discussion, but she made a point of almost being candid in sections. Were you able to feel a stronger connection with an author in this manner as opposed to those authors that merely put words to a page and don’t narrate their own work? Was this similarly attempted by Martin and colleagues? If it wasn’t, do you think it could have been an approach that would have benefited their structure? Do you think this type of writing can almost be seen as a narrative? Any thoughts?

  3. Hi Kat,

    Thanks for your comments! I’m not sure if the presence of the author in her writing is the reason that I enjoyed Agarwal’s (2012) article so much, but I think that this definitely contributed to the flow of the article and made it easier to understand and more pleasant to read. She was very candid in terms of telling the story of publishing her results, and I thought that this really added to the portrayal of how established current methods are and how resistant the majority of researchers are to changing how things are done and conceptualized. This could definitely be a factor in encouraging a closer connection between the author and her readers.
    I don’t think that this was similarly attempted by Martin et al. (2010). However, I don’t know that this approach would have been able to overcome the major limitations of the article. It may however have contributed to improvements in the flow and connection between various sections of the article.
    I do see aspects of narrative writing in both of the articles, especially in how they trace the development of various theoretical concepts through previous scholarship to the present and their own analyses. I think that the article by Agarwal (2012) contains additional smaller narratives, including as mentioned above her quest for publication, on top of and within the broader progression of her argument. In my opinion, what Agarwal (2012) does well that falls short in other articles including the paper by Martin et al. (2010) is tie all aspects of these narratives together into one cohesive argument that flows consistently rather than relying on the reader’s ability to follow where “interpretive leaps” have been made by the authors.

  4. Excellent reviews Laura. I have to say, I am pretty biased here, as I know Sabrina quite well (I was the graduate student on her hiring committee at Berkeley, and TA’d her Biological Anthropology class for several semesters). I watched Sabrina begin her “salsa dance” with great encouragement from theoretical archaeologists (such as Rosemary Joyce, who we read earlier in the semester). I agree with you — she is playing the boundaries in a very interesting way.

    You all are also highlighting a key issue in Anthropological writing concerning theory. The term theory is being bandied about on the blogs this week as if it were something that you could not do. Even when you claim to be “atheoretical”, theory is still in there, but likely working in ways you are unaware. Theory is about your assumptions and your understandings of how the world works. Being explicit about it (and yes, this sometimes requires jargon!) means that your “taken-for-granteds”, and their histories, are highlighted.

    Laura (and others’ on their blogs) is highlighting that bad articles are often bad as the “theory laden facts” don’t fit the theoretical framework being developed.** And what makes Agarwal’s writing exciting is that she is highlighting assumptions, and playing with them to show how data might be theoretically informed in unexpected ways. What do you think?

    ** I should point out (take note you PhD candidates!) this is also one of the hardest things to do in both proposal writing, but also thesis writing.

Leave a Reply

Your email address will not be published. Required fields are marked *

*